This issue dives into the study of sleep function, particularly as it relates to memory and cognition. Any clinician who sees patients with sleep disorders, or in particular any sleep medicine specialist, will find this information enlightening and invaluable, as it discusses the current state of understanding of how sleep affects humans' waking cognitive functions. These review articles describe the research that has taken place, and the lessons that can be taken away from them, so that clinicians can confidently advise their patients on the functional importance of adequate sleep, and recognize cognitive symptoms of inadequate sleep. Articles discuss such topics as animal and human research on sleep and memory, various imaging techniques to describe brain activity during sleep, and the role of dreams.
Harvard Medical School
Front Cover 1
Sleep, Memory, and Learning 2
Copyright Page 3
Sleep Medicine Clinics 5
Contributors 6
Contents 8
Foreword 12
Preface: Sleep, Learning, and Memory 14
Chapter 1. The Neurology of Sleep 18
HISTORICAL CONTEXT 18
SLEEP AND WAKE STATES 19
ASCENDING RETICULAR SYSTEM 21
CIRCADIAN RHYTHM 25
HOMESTATIC AND CIRCADIAN SLEEP-WAKE INTERACTIONS 26
FORCED DESYNCHRONY 27
AGENTS OF ENTRAINMENT 28
REFERENCES 29
Chapter 2. Learning, Memory, and Sleep in Humans 32
DEFINING SLEEP AND MEMORY 32
SLEEP BENEFITS IMPLICIT AND PROCEDURAL MEMORIES 33
SLEEP BENEFITS EXPLICIT MEMORIES 38
SLEEP BENEFITS EMOTIONAL MEMORY CONSOLIDATION 39
SLEEP TRANSFORMS MEMORIES IN USEFUL WAYS 40
BEYOND SLEEP STAGES 41
NEUROPHYSIOLOGICAL AND NEUROCHEMICAL EVIDENCE FOR SLEEP’S ROLE IN MEMORY CONSOLIDATION 41
NEUROTRANSMITTERS AND NEUROHORMONES 42
FUTURE DIRECTIONS 44
REFERENCES 44
Chapter 3. Sleep and Emotional Memory Processing 48
SLEEP 48
SLEEP LOSS, MOOD STABILITY, AND EMOTIONAL BRAIN (RE)ACTIVITY 53
A HEURISTIC MODEL OF SLEEP-DEPENDENT EMOTIONAL PROCESSING 55
EMOTIONAL MEMORY PROCESSING: A SLEEP TO FORGET AND SLEEP TO REMEMBER HYPOTHESIS 55
SUMMARY 58
REFERENCES 58
Chapter 4. Learning, Memory, and Sleep in Children 62
DEVELOPMENT OF SLEEP 62
CORRELATIONAL RELATIONSHIPS BETWEEN SLEEP AND LEARNING 64
THE EFFECTS OF SLEEP RESTRICTION ON CHILDHOOD LEARNING 67
DIRECT COMPARISONS OF MEMORY CONSOLIDATION DURING WAKE AND SLEEP 68
SUMMARY 70
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 71
REFERENCES 71
Chapter 5. Sleep States and Memory Processing in Rodents: A Review 76
REM SLEEP AND MEMORY 77
THE REM SLEEP GENERATOR 78
MEMORY ENHANCEMENT 78
SWS AND MEMORY 79
SWS AND REPLAY 79
REM SLEEP AND REPLAY 81
EMOTIONAL MEMORY 82
NEUROCHEMICAL AND GENETIC FACTORS 82
STATUS OF SLEEP-MEMORY THEORIES 84
FUTURE DIRECTIONS 84
REFERENCES 84
Chapyer 6. A Molecular Basis for Interactions Between Sleep and Memory 88
STAGES OF MEMORY PROCESSING 88
SLEEP STATES AND MEMORY 93
HOMEOSTATIC AND CIRCADIAN CONTROL OF SLEEP-DEPENDENT MEMORY PROCESSING 94
MOLECULAR REPLAY AND SLEEP-DEPENDENT MEMORY PROCESSING 95
THE USE OF GENETIC MODELS TO STUDY SLEEP-MEMORY INTERACTIONS 96
SUMMARY 96
REFERENCES 96
Chapter 7. Brain Stimulation During Sleep 102
HISTORICAL PERSPECTIVE AND STIMULATION PROTOCOLS 102
STIMULATING THE BRAIN DURING SWS 103
TMS STIMULATION TO PROBE BRAIN EXCITABILITY DURING SLEEP 106
BRAIN STIMULATION DURING SLEEP, MEMORY, AND HOMEOSTATIC REGULATION 107
SLEEP AFTER TMS-INDUCED PLASTICITY 109
SUMMARY 109
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 109
REFERENCES 110
Chapter 8. Memory, Sleep, and Dreaming: Experiencing Consolidation 114
MEMORIES IN THE SLEEPING BRAIN 114
THE INCORPORATION OF RECENT EXPERIENCE INTO DREAMING 117
THE EMPIRICAL STUDY OF SPONTANEOUS SUBJECTIVE EXPERIENCE 121
REACTIVATION OF MEMORY IN DREAM CONTENT AND SLEEP-DEPENDENT MEMORY CONSOLIDATION 122
SUMMARY 123
REFERENCES 123
Chapter 9. The Neurocognitive Effects of Sleep Disruption in Children and Adolescents 126
CULTURAL 126
POOR SLEEP HYGIENE/INSUFFICIENT SLEEP 127
DELAYED SLEEP PHASE SYNDROME 128
SLEEP-DISORDERED BREATHING 128
RESTLESS LEGS SYNDROME/PERIODIC LIMB MOVEMENTS 130
NARCOLEPSY 130
TREATMENT OF SLEEP PROBLEMS 130
IS THERE A COMMON THREAD? 131
SUMMARY 131
REFERENCES 131
Chapter 10. Sleepy Driving 134
SLEEPINESS AND DRIVING: A BRIEF REVIEW 134
BRIEF HISTORY OF SLEEPINESS 134
THE NATIONAL COMMISSION ON SLEEP 136
ALCOHOL VERSUS SLEEPY DRIVING: A COMPARATIVE MODEL 136
SLEEPY DRIVING ACCIDENT RISKS 138
SUMMARY 139
REFERENCES 139
Index 142
Clinics in Sleep Medicine, Vol. 6, No. 1, March 2011
ISSN: 1556-407X
doi: 10.1016/j.jsmc.2010.12.009
The Neurology of Sleep
a School of Medicine, University of Texas-Houston, Houston, TX, USA
b The Houston Sleep Center, Houston, TX, USA
* University of Texas-Houston, 7500 San Felipe, Suite 525, Houston, TX 77063.
E-mail address: tswick@houstonsleepcenter.com
Abstract
Neurology, by virtue of its study of the brain, is the primary medical science for the elucidation of the anatomy, physiology, pathology and ultimately, the function of sleep. There has been nothing short of a revolution in the science of sleep over the past 50 years. From the discovery of REM sleep to the identification of Hypocretin/Orexin the basic science and clinical field of sleep medicine has blossomed. This article will explore the anatomy, physiology, biochemistry and, to a limited extent, pathophysiology of the sleep/wake centers of the brain. The field of chronobiology will also be touched upon.
Keywords
• Acetylcholine • Serotonin • Dopamine • Norepinephrine • Histamine • Orexin • Melatonin • Circadian rhythm
Neurology, by virtue of its study of the brain, is the primary medical science for the elucidation of the anatomy, physiology, pathology, and, ultimately, the function of sleep.
Historical context
The Greco-Roman concepts of sleep were based on their belief that there were gods and goddesses that controlled the minor and major events of their lives. They identified the goddess of night (Nyx) who had two sons: Hypnos (the god of sleep) and his brother, Thanantos (the god of death). Hypnos sprinkled drops of poppy milk into people’s eyes so that the opium would make them fall asleep and then fanned sleeping persons with his wings to enable them to sleep in comfort. As late as the beginning of the Common Era, Ovid wrote that Hypnos lived with his “thousand children,” the Dreams, in a cave in the Caucasus. The river of Lethe (the river of forgetfulness) was believed to run through this cave.1
In ancient Greece, if citizens were unable to sleep because of their problems, they visited one of the many sanitariums dedicated to Asclepios (the Greek god of medicine), where the afflicted spent 3 weeks in rest, thought, and meditation, soothed by gentle music, and then, having their balance restored, would be able to sleep again (obviously predating the concept of managed care).2
From the time of the Middle Ages until the Renaissance, there were discrete changes in the concept of sleep. More concrete explanations of sleep were enunciated by Lucretius, the Epicurean poet and philosopher, when he described “sleep as the absence of wakefulness”.3 This was the prevailing view through the centuries. As medical science advanced with the discovery of the circulatory system and as the young field of neurology was explored, there was renewed interest in the science of sleep and wakefulness.
In 1866, the Surgeon General of the United States, William A. Hammond, wrote a treatise, “On Wakefulness: With an Introductory Chapter on the Physiology of Sleep,” arguing against the prevailing opinion of his day that sleep began as a consequence of “congestion of the cerebral vessels.” He pointed out several observations that were quoted in contemporary textbooks of medical physiology of his time: (1) stupor never occurs in healthy individuals, whereas sleep is a necessity of life; (2) it is easy to awaken a person from sleep, whereas it often is impossible to arouse him from stupor; (3) in sleep the mind is active and in stupor it is as if it were dead; and (4) congestion of cerebral vessels causes stupor, not sleep. He quotes another nineteenth-century physician, Dr Arthur Durham: “During sleep, the brain is in a comparatively bloodless condition and the blood in the encephalic vessels is not only diminished in quantity but moves with diminished rapidity. Whatever increases the activity of the cerebral circulation tends to preserve wakefulness and whatever decreases the activity of the cerebral circulation and, at the same time, is not inconsistent with the general health of the body tends to induce and favor sleep”.4
Although still surrounded by myth and less than perfect science, the concept of the neural control of sleep was established. From 1916 through 1928, the world was ravaged by an epidemic of influenza with tens of thousands of deaths, the victims sustaining many neurologic signs and symptoms. During the acute phase of the illness, some patients exhibited severe insomnia and many more exhibited severe hypersomnia, whereas many survivors exhibited signs of Parkinsonism.
In 1917, von Economo published his first paper on encephalitis lethargica and on December 3, 1929, he read a paper before the College of Physicians and Surgeons of Columbia University in New York City entitled, “Sleep as a Problem of Localization.” He stated that patients who had insomnia had lesions in the anterior portion of their hypothalamus and that patients who had hypersomnia had lesions in the posterior aspect of the hypothalamus. He designated this area of the “interbrain” as the “center for regulation of sleep.” Thus, the prevailing concept espoused by such luminaries as Lhermitte and Dejerine that “sleep cannot be localized” was put to rest.5,6
In 1928, Berger demonstrated that the brain produced clearly identifiable electrical activity that could be recorded using surface electrodes and that there existed a different pattern of electrical activity of the brain during consciousness compared with sleep.6,7
In 1935, Bremer reported on the effects of transection of the brainstem of cats at the pontine-midbrain level (cerveau isolé) versus transection at the medullary-spinal cord level (encéphale isolé). He found that the cerveau isolé animals maintained a continuous sleep-like state with synchronous slow wave activity, whereas the encéphale isolé cats looked awake and their electroencephalograms (EEGs) contained synchronous and desynchronized activity resembling sleep-wake cycling. Bremer went on to hypothesize that sleep was a passive process and that wakefulness required a high level of continuous sensory input from the periphery to maintain activity within the cerebral hemispheres.8
Bremer’s work rekindled research concerning the observations of y Cajal and Papez. In 1909 y Cajal described an extensive network of neurons that ascended and descended through the brainstem. This was refined further by observations of Papez who in 1926 published a more complete description of the reticular formation and its caudal projections down into the spinal cord in cats.9,10
In 1942, Morison and Dempsey published a series of articles that described a diffuse “nonspecific” thalamocortical recruiting system. They differentiated this “nonspecific” system from the primary sensory input (ie, “specific” system [described by Lorente de No in 1938], acting through direct thalamic relays).11,12
In 1949, Moruzzi and Magoun identified the ascending reticular activating system “whose direct stimulation activates or desynchronizes the EEG, replacing high-voltage slow waves with low-voltage fast activity.” They went on to state, “the effect is exerted generally upon the cortex and is mediated, in part, at least, by the diffuse thalamic projection system”.13
By the middle of the twentieth century it was established that sleep and wakefulness were different states that are controlled by the brain and that sleep was not a passive period of time devoid of activity. Jouvet and colleagues described more precise localizations of the neural loci of sleep and its constituents where results of lesion studies demonstrated that the brainstem contains the site of rapid eye movement (REM) sleep neural activity. Transections of a cat brain at a level just above the midbrain-pons junction preserved the appearance of REM-sleep activity, whereas transections in the pons abolished the appearance of REM sleep.14-16
With the discoveries of REM and non-REM (NREM) sleep by Aserinsky and Kleitman and REM/NREM cycling by Dement and Kleitman, the door finally was opened for researchers to gain more exact insights into the study of the science of sleep and wakefulness.17-19
Sleep and wake states
People exist in one of three behavioral states during normal functioning: (1) wakefulness; (2) NREM sleep; and (3) REM sleep (Fig. 1). These states are characterized by specific changes in EEG, eye movements, and muscle activity. Wakefulness is characterized by well-recognized patterns on surface EEG recording. Alpha activity (8–12 Hz waves of <50 μV amplitude) occurs when individuals are resting with their eyes closed. The rhythms are most evident in the parieto-occipital areas of the head. Alpha rhythm is attenuated or blocked by attention, especially visual (eye-opening) and mental effort. Eye movements are purposeful and conjugate. Muscle tone is variable but never absent.
Fig. 1 EEG...
Erscheint lt. Verlag | 21.3.2011 |
---|---|
Sprache | englisch |
Themenwelt | Medizin / Pharmazie ► Gesundheitsfachberufe |
Medizinische Fachgebiete ► Innere Medizin ► Pneumologie | |
ISBN-10 | 1-4557-0954-9 / 1455709549 |
ISBN-13 | 978-1-4557-0954-0 / 9781455709540 |
Haben Sie eine Frage zum Produkt? |
Größe: 4,7 MB
Kopierschutz: Adobe-DRM
Adobe-DRM ist ein Kopierschutz, der das eBook vor Mißbrauch schützen soll. Dabei wird das eBook bereits beim Download auf Ihre persönliche Adobe-ID autorisiert. Lesen können Sie das eBook dann nur auf den Geräten, welche ebenfalls auf Ihre Adobe-ID registriert sind.
Details zum Adobe-DRM
Dateiformat: PDF (Portable Document Format)
Mit einem festen Seitenlayout eignet sich die PDF besonders für Fachbücher mit Spalten, Tabellen und Abbildungen. Eine PDF kann auf fast allen Geräten angezeigt werden, ist aber für kleine Displays (Smartphone, eReader) nur eingeschränkt geeignet.
Systemvoraussetzungen:
PC/Mac: Mit einem PC oder Mac können Sie dieses eBook lesen. Sie benötigen eine
eReader: Dieses eBook kann mit (fast) allen eBook-Readern gelesen werden. Mit dem amazon-Kindle ist es aber nicht kompatibel.
Smartphone/Tablet: Egal ob Apple oder Android, dieses eBook können Sie lesen. Sie benötigen eine
Geräteliste und zusätzliche Hinweise
Zusätzliches Feature: Online Lesen
Dieses eBook können Sie zusätzlich zum Download auch online im Webbrowser lesen.
Buying eBooks from abroad
For tax law reasons we can sell eBooks just within Germany and Switzerland. Regrettably we cannot fulfill eBook-orders from other countries.
Kopierschutz: Adobe-DRM
Adobe-DRM ist ein Kopierschutz, der das eBook vor Mißbrauch schützen soll. Dabei wird das eBook bereits beim Download auf Ihre persönliche Adobe-ID autorisiert. Lesen können Sie das eBook dann nur auf den Geräten, welche ebenfalls auf Ihre Adobe-ID registriert sind.
Details zum Adobe-DRM
Dateiformat: EPUB (Electronic Publication)
EPUB ist ein offener Standard für eBooks und eignet sich besonders zur Darstellung von Belletristik und Sachbüchern. Der Fließtext wird dynamisch an die Display- und Schriftgröße angepasst. Auch für mobile Lesegeräte ist EPUB daher gut geeignet.
Systemvoraussetzungen:
PC/Mac: Mit einem PC oder Mac können Sie dieses eBook lesen. Sie benötigen eine
eReader: Dieses eBook kann mit (fast) allen eBook-Readern gelesen werden. Mit dem amazon-Kindle ist es aber nicht kompatibel.
Smartphone/Tablet: Egal ob Apple oder Android, dieses eBook können Sie lesen. Sie benötigen eine
Geräteliste und zusätzliche Hinweise
Buying eBooks from abroad
For tax law reasons we can sell eBooks just within Germany and Switzerland. Regrettably we cannot fulfill eBook-orders from other countries.
aus dem Bereich