Nicht aus der Schweiz? Besuchen Sie lehmanns.de

Person and Number (eBook)

An Empirical Study of Catalan Sign Language Pronouns
eBook Download: EPUB
2023
256 Seiten
De Gruyter (Verlag)
978-3-11-098919-9 (ISBN)

Lese- und Medienproben

Person and Number - Raquel Veiga Busto
Systemvoraussetzungen
144,95 inkl. MwSt
(CHF 139,95)
Der eBook-Verkauf erfolgt durch die Lehmanns Media GmbH (Berlin) zum Preis in Euro inkl. MwSt.
  • Download sofort lieferbar
  • Zahlungsarten anzeigen
Person and number are two basic grammatical categories. However, they have not yet been exhaustively documented in many sign languages. This volume presents a thorough description of the form and interpretation of person and number in Catalan Sign Language (LSC) personal pronouns. This is the first book exploring together the two categories (and their interaction) in a sign language.
Building on a combination of elicitation methods and corpus data analysis, this book shows that person and number are encoded through a set of distinctive phonological features: person is formally marked through spatial features, and number by the path specifications of the sign. Additionally, this study provides evidence that the same number marker might have a different semantic import depending on the person features with which it is combined.
Results of this investigation contribute fresh data to cross-linguistic studies on person and number, which are largely based on evidence from spoken language only. Furthermore, while this research identifies a number of significant differences with respect to prior descriptions of person and number in other sign languages, it also demonstrates that, from a typological standpoint, the array of distinctions that LSC draws within each category is not exceptional.

Raquel Veiga Busto, Universität Amsterdam, Niederlande.

Part I: Person


The most striking fact about deictic phenomena in sign languages may turn out to be the lack of significant modality effects.

(Berenz 1996: 305)

The three chapters that follow aim to investigate whether the person category is grammaticalized in LSC. Chapter 2 sets the ground by discussing the concept of person and the distinctions made in this category in the world’s (signed and spoken) languages. Chapter 3 turns to the case of LSC personal pronouns. It presents a thorough description of the morphophonological strategies used in this language to express reference to the participants in the speech act. The description is based on the analysis of nearly 1,300 pointing signs produced by 18 different signers, and it is further complemented with elicited material. The study shows that, in LSC, i) each person value is systematically associated with different morphophonological markers and ii) that such form-meaning correspondence is found in both singular and non-singular (dual and plural) pronouns. Finally, Chapter 4 puts forward an analysis that captures the articulatory distinctions encoding person values described in Chapter 3. The core of the proposal relies on the notion of ‘spatial features’, which is understood as a set of distinctive phonological features that stand for the way pronouns are projected into the signing space. The main claim of Part I is that spatial locations are grammatically relevant in the expression and interpretation of person distinctions in LSC, thus supporting a grammatical analysis of the signing space.

2 The person category


The category of person encodes the semantic distinction between discourse roles. It is typically expressed on pronouns and verbs, while nouns are generally assumed to have an inherent third person value (Corbett 2006a). In Part I of this book, I will focus on the expression of person in personal pronouns only.6

The goal of this chapter is to present the most common distinctions drawn within the person category in the world’s (spoken) languages. Since cross-linguistic studies only very rarely discuss the person distinctions encoded in sign language grammars, I will tackle this issue in the second part of the chapter.

In sign languages, reference to the participants in the speech act is deeply connected with the use of the signing space, but accounts of the association between spatial locations and referents are far from uniform. This translates into different analyses of the person category, as well as into diverging proposals regarding the number of person values claimed to be grammatically encoded in sign language grammars.

This chapter is organized as follows. Section 2.1 introduces the notion of pronominal person, the most common distinctions drawn in the person category and their relation to the number category. Section 2.2 offers an overview of the cross-linguistic tendencies observed in the category of person in the world’s (spoken) languages. In Section 2.3, I turn to sign languages and report on the main proposals that account for the association of spatial location and referents, as well as for the expression of person distinctions in sign language pronouns.

2.1 Person distinctions


2.1.1 Participants vs. non-participants

The grammatical category of person encodes, both in the verbal and in the nominal domain, reference to the participants in the speech act. First and second person express the conversational roles of the speech act participants: first person refers (minimally) to the speaker and second person to the addressee. The nonparticipants in the speech act, in turn, are associated with third person.

The participants vs. non-participants split is based on the observation that the third person is fundamentally distinct from the first and the second (cf. Forchheimer 1953; Benveniste 1971; Lyons 1977; a.o.). According to Benveniste (1971: 217) “[…] the ordinary definition of the personal pronouns as containing the three terms I, you, and he simply destroys the notion of ‘person’. ‘Person’ belongs only to I/you and is lacking in he”. Similarly, Lyons (1977: 638) notes that “[t]he term ‘third person’ is negatively defined with respect to ‘first person’ and ‘second person’: it does not correlate with any positive participant role”. This is why third person is also referred as ‘the non-person’, ‘third party’ or ‘the other category’ (Benveniste 1971; Cysouw 2001; Siewierska 2004; a.o.).

The first/second vs. third person distinction is borne out by considering both semantic and morphosyntactic evidence. For example, third person pronouns are more likely to show gender distinctions than first and second person pronouns. Number distinctions, by contrast, are more commonly neutralized in the third person (see Sections 2.2 and 7.4.2). Besides, some languages lack dedicated markers to express reference to the non-participants, which may translate into absence of specialized third person pronouns or into zero verbal agreement. For instance, the Nakho-Daghestanian language Lezgian, spoken in the Caucasus, uses the substantivized demonstrative am (‘that one’) instead of a third person pronoun, as illustrated in (3). Alternatively, languages may express reference to the nonparticipant by using full noun phrases or zero forms (i.e., no overt expression whatsoever) (Siewierska 2004).7

(3) Lezgian (Haspelmath 1993: 184)
SG
1 zun ‘I’ (personal pronoun)
2 wun ‘you’ (personal pronoun)
3 am ‘that one’ (demonstrative)

Zero agreement in the third person is fairly common in the world’s languages, unlike zero agreement in first and second person (Cysouw 2001). This is the case of the Mongolian language Buriat in (4). In the singular, overt agreement suffixes, which are derived from personal pronouns, are used in first and second person. Absence of a person suffix, in turn, is associated with reference to the non-participant. In the plural, the third person suffix is derived from the demonstrative ede (‘these’).

(4) Buriat (Poppe 1960, cited in Bybee 2015: 153)
SG PL
1 -b -bdi
2 -s -t
3 ø -d

An additional distinction between first and second person pronouns as opposed to the third is that the former are inherently deictic elements and, as such, their interpretation shifts with the speaker (i.e., they belong to the class of ‘shifters’, cf. Jespersen 1922; Jakobson 1971). By contrast, third person pronouns may have both indexical (i.e., deictic) and non-indexical (i.e., anaphoric) uses (Kaplan 1989).8The interpretation of anaphoric third person pronouns does not shift, as they are bound to their antecedents, not to the extralinguistic context.9

Notationally, the reference of person is labeled using either Arabic numbers (1 for speaker, 2 for addressee, 3 for other; cf. Zwicky 1977) or, in the tradition of Harbour (2016) and Ackema & Neeleman (2018), using letters (i for speaker, u for addressee, o for other). Here, I will follow the more widespread convention of representing the...

Erscheint lt. Verlag 4.7.2023
Reihe/Serie ISSN
ISSN
Sign Languages and Deaf Communities [SLDC]
Sign Languages and Deaf Communities [SLDC]
Zusatzinfo 85 b/w ill., 40 b/w tbl.
Sprache englisch
Themenwelt Schulbuch / Wörterbuch Wörterbuch / Fremdsprachen
Geisteswissenschaften Sprach- / Literaturwissenschaft Anglistik / Amerikanistik
Schlagworte Catalan Sign Language • Katalanische Gebärdensprache • Llengua de Signes Catalana • Number (Grammar) • Numerus • Person (Grammar) • Person / Grammatik
ISBN-10 3-11-098919-0 / 3110989190
ISBN-13 978-3-11-098919-9 / 9783110989199
Haben Sie eine Frage zum Produkt?
EPUBEPUB (Wasserzeichen)
Größe: 19,5 MB

DRM: Digitales Wasserzeichen
Dieses eBook enthält ein digitales Wasser­zeichen und ist damit für Sie persona­lisiert. Bei einer missbräuch­lichen Weiter­gabe des eBooks an Dritte ist eine Rück­ver­folgung an die Quelle möglich.

Dateiformat: EPUB (Electronic Publication)
EPUB ist ein offener Standard für eBooks und eignet sich besonders zur Darstellung von Belle­tristik und Sach­büchern. Der Fließ­text wird dynamisch an die Display- und Schrift­größe ange­passt. Auch für mobile Lese­geräte ist EPUB daher gut geeignet.

Systemvoraussetzungen:
PC/Mac: Mit einem PC oder Mac können Sie dieses eBook lesen. Sie benötigen dafür die kostenlose Software Adobe Digital Editions.
eReader: Dieses eBook kann mit (fast) allen eBook-Readern gelesen werden. Mit dem amazon-Kindle ist es aber nicht kompatibel.
Smartphone/Tablet: Egal ob Apple oder Android, dieses eBook können Sie lesen. Sie benötigen dafür eine kostenlose App.
Geräteliste und zusätzliche Hinweise

Buying eBooks from abroad
For tax law reasons we can sell eBooks just within Germany and Switzerland. Regrettably we cannot fulfill eBook-orders from other countries.

Mehr entdecken
aus dem Bereich
The Structure of Disorder in the Anatomy of Melancholy

von Ruth A. Fox

eBook Download (2023)
University of California Press (Verlag)
CHF 48,80